Beautiful Wednesday
I woke up too lately this morning..
But i did not leave my class, advance listening.
I got number 4 to practice, it was the best number than other..I hope it could make my mark better. Amin
Pages
On the Origin of Language
In the Western world the study of language began as a philosophical inquiry into origins.1 The
Greeks (Third and Fourth Century B.C.) initiated the study of language
essentially to explain its origin. The Conventionalists hypothesized
that the relationship between the form of language (i.e., primarily the
sounds and words) and meaning was essentially arbitrary, a convention of
society. The naturalists hypothesized that the form of a word (i.e.,
its sounds) had a natural association with its referent in the real
world. Only certain sound combinations (words or parts of words),
however, were directly associated as an imitation of an object, its
sound or an idea directly associated as an imitation of an object (e.g.,
kookaburra).
In an effort to explain how most of language, which is not so
directly relatable to meaning, derived from an onomatopoeic beginning,
the discipline of etymology began. Through studying the derivational
history of words (etymology) the naturalists intended to demonstrate
that the origin of all of language was ultimately relatable to words
which directly reflected the meanings of their referents.
The first philosophical forum on language eventually developed into a
discussion on the regularity of language patterns. Two basic
theoretical positions emerged as explanatory frameworks for language,
that which opted for irregularity and that which insisted that language
was essentially regular. From the pre-eminence of the latter position it
became popular to explain the irregularities of language on the basis
that language somehow became corrupted with improper usage through time;
this theoretical position regarded the older forms of language to be
the purer forms.
By the Nineteenth Century there was a severe reaction to the highly
speculative nature of the philosophizing about the original language of
man which had characterized much of the study of language up until then.
The interest was still historical, but the goal was not so idealistic.
It was a romantic era of a rediscovery of the national past; the mother
tongues of nations and families of nations rather than the mother tongue
of the whole human race became the focus of attention. The romantic
nationalism was a definite influence, but perhaps a more basic cause of
the more realistic goal was the reaction to previous unscientific
speculations. The felt need was to take a more scientific approach by
analyzing empirical data. Thus was ushered in the period of systematic
comparison of languages for the purpose of reconstructing the historical
past.
During the Nineteenth Century largely under the leadership of German
scholars an impressive amount of detailed scholarly work was done.
Building on Sir William Jones' discovery that Sanskrit was genetically
related to Latin and Greek and other European languages as well, these
early historical linguists began to develop principles of language
comparison. The availability of historical data not only made possible
advances in the reconstruction of the original Indo-European language2
(proto Indo-European), it also enabled linguists to describe the
processes of change by which the proto-language developed into the
diversity of the many Indo-European languages.
The German based 'neo-grammarian' school is known for its
contribution to the study of sound change in the last quarter of the
Nineteenth Century. The neo-grammarians, through meticulous analysis of
historical text material, demonstrated the striking regularity of sound
change. Hermann Paul (1846 -- 1921), the foremost theoretician of the
neo-grammarians, identified convenience as the central mechanism of
sound change; within the framework of convenience he categorized three
types of sound changes under the mode of 'mispronunciation.'3
Leonard Bloomfield (1887-- 1949) was an early American structuralist
who extended the neo-grammarian position with greater detail. He
catalogued the mechanisms of sound change as two types: stabilizing vs.
deteriorating or simplifying mechanisms. He documented at least three
stabilizing changes characterized as reformation and compensatory
processes. In the simplifying category Bloomfield documented no less
than eleven processes of sound change.
Detailed documentation by the neo-grammarians of various processes of
language change, especially those of sound change, contributed greatly
to the statement of two basic principles of language change, 1) the
process of streamlining and 2) the process of restructuring. Martinet,
one of the most eminent historical linguists in the Twentieth Century,
is credited with formulating these two principles of language change. He
refers to the restructuring process, which maintains adequate
communication, as being in conflict with the streamlining process which
manifests (in language) the human tendency toward reducing effort to a
minimum.
Linguistic evolution may be regarded as governed by the permanent conflict between man's communicative needs and his tendency to reduce to a minimum his mental and physical activity. Here as elsewhere, human behavior is subject to the law of least effort. (Martinet 1964:167).
The law of least effort effects a relentless streamlining of the
status quo, reducing complexity and redundancy, which in turn eventually
leads to restructuring adjustments in the various systems of language
to help maintain an acceptable level of communication. The restructuring
principle could be termed the law of conservation of communication.
It would be misleading to imply that Hermann Paul was not interested
in the origin of language; the question of origin certainly interested
Paul as it still does linguists today. The essential difference between
modern linguistics (the past 175 years) and that of the two previous
millennia is that linguistics has moved from the purely philosophical
realm to that of the empirical sciences. Linguists are still intrigued
with the question of origins but their speculations on the origin of
language must be based on observable facts about language.
Two important basic principles of language must be mentioned, the
streamlining effect of least effort and the compensatory maintenance of
communication, or restructuring; two observations that relate to these
are worth noting.
a) Primitive languages: No group of human beings today, even those
living in a stone-age culture, speak what could be conceived of as a
primitive language. Furthermore, no known language in all of history was
in any sense primitive. Elgin remarks, 'The most ancient languages for
which we have written texts -- Sanskrit, for example -- are often far
more intricate and complicated in their grammatical forms than many
contemporary languages.' (Elgin 1973: 44) This, of course, is no
surprise to us if the inevitable processes of simplification observable
today have consistently been operating for all or most of human history
(this is in itself of course indeterminate, but we can at least conclude
that simple material cultures do not imply simple languages).
b) Creativity of language: The vocabulary may be considered to be the
most creative area of language and even here, 'For the most part,
people tend to re-adapt existing lexical material rather than create
entirely new material.' (Langacker 1967:186). Apart from re-adapting and
extending existing vocabulary items from within a particular language,
words or parts of words are commonly borrowed from other languages. A
language seldom exhibits creativity in the sense of inventing new and
unique forms.
The English pronoun system illustrates the two basic principles in
action today. The oldest English pronoun system distinguished three
numbers (singular, dual, plural) for each of 1st, 2nd, and 3rd persons.
Today standard English distinguishes only singular and plural. The
previously 'extravagant' system has been streamlined by neutralizing the
difference between duality and plurality. In addition, with the 2nd
person 'you' the singular-plural distinction has been lost, resulting in
unacceptable ambiguity (ineffective communication) at times. (One of
the first times I asked the girl who is now my wife for a date I ended
up taking a whole carload of people on an outing because she wasn't sure
whether I meant 'you-singular' or ‘you-plural’; I was too embarrassed
to expressly exclude everyone else who was there at the time. As far as I
was concerned, that was an unacceptable ambiguity!) The restructuring
presently going on in English to remedy this situation involves adding
particles from elsewhere in the grammar into the pronoun system. A new
2nd-person-plural pronoun is being formed in the northeastern United
States by adding the normal noun pluralizer "s" to the pronoun "you"
resulting in the plural "yous" (pronounced the same as "ewes"). The
much-publicized "Southern dialect" has restructured the system in
another way. A phrase-level quantifier "all" has been added in a
contacted form of "you-all" resulting in "y'all." In both cases the
restructuring process is clearly adaptive rather than innovative.
Many linguists, apparently including Martinet, believe that the two
opposing principles equalize each other. Langacker states, 'Just as
there are no primitive languages, there are no 'corrupt' languages.
Languages change, but they do not decay.' (Langacker 1973:17) This is a
difficult point to verify. Support for this claim seems to have been
well documented in a historical review of the sound system of Spanish.
While sounds have changed, the number of distinctive features in the
system have remained fairly constant; from the viewpoint of information
processing the overall process of change has not altered the potential
for communication. Many of Bloomfield's examples of simplification due
to sound change appear to affect syntactic categories and vocabulary.
The loss of noun case endings in English is a case in point. The
relative complexity of communication potential of the resulting system
is difficult to evaluate. While the case ending functions of identifying
subjects, objects, etc. have been shifted to another level in grammar
(word order on the clause level now usually identifies the subject and
object) it is difficult to judge how the two systems compare in
efficiency of communication. Some questions to be asked would be, 'What
happened to previous functions of word order in clauses?', 'Is word
order now overloaded with the jobs of case-role encoding and the
indicating of old and new information in a discourse?', 'Is focus
involved?'
Language is fantastically complex. Its built-in means of combining
and recombining (nesting) of its various levels has suggested to many
leading linguists that language is theoretically infinite though not
practically so in everyday usage. It almost sounds too complex to be
able to detect any significant leveling out of language any more than
one could detect by observation that the sun is burning itself out.
As far as I am aware no linguist seriously purports that the
restructuring process of language overrides the streamlining process
resulting in a qualitative positive development of language. If we
decide that language did originally develop, possibly evolving from
animal communication, we can only do so assuming evolution to be a
universally valid principle. This type of a priori reasoning
was the basic fallacy of pre-Nineteenth Century 'speculative grammar'
which was pre-scientific in the modern sense of the word.
Furthermore, the observable data neither indicate that such a period
of pre-historic development even existed, nor do they suggest a cause of
the subsequent state of equilibrium or process of simplification that
would have to have come into operation at some time after such a
pre-historic development. Noam Chomsky, one of the most prominent
linguists of this century, has indicated that human language and animal
communication are not even comparable entities, they are so different.
Either the streamlining and restructuring processes balance each
other or the streamlining process is gradually reducing language to a
limited system of over-generalities. Either human languages have always
existed with essentially the potential they exhibit now or they once
exhibited greater potential for precise communication than they now do.
Labov, a prominent contemporary sociolinguist, comments on these two
processes in his effort to understand the place of language in an
evolutionary framework:
It is plain to most linguists that the 'destroy and rebuild' theory
of linguistic evolution is equivalent to claiming that the whole process
is dysfunctional. For the systematic part is the destructive one, and
the analogical re-shaping seems to be making the best of a bad job. And
if the principle of least effort is the evil genius behind the
destruction, we can only look at language change as a kind of massive
testimony to original sin. (Labov 1973: 245)
In the remainder of his thesis Labov does not provide any relevant
alternative to the dysfunctional role of language change. He does not
deny that language change results in diversification of languages and
not in overall complexity or adaptive radiation. He rather looks for
functional evolutionary result in the development of human society. Thus
he suggests that language diversity provides relative cultural
isolation, maintaining cultural pluralism which presumably promotes the
evolution of human society. But he cannot provide a functional role for
language change purely within the development of language. Language
development seems to be dominated by a dysfunctional (non-evolutionary)
process. Conclusion:
Regardless of how we might attempt to fit language into the broader
picture, looking at language by itself there is no evidence that
language is the product of any positive developmental process. Language
is in a state of consistent change which at best seems to maintain a
state of equilibrium.
__________________________________
1 The Indian tradition antedates the work of the Greeks. Panini (Fourth Century B.C.) culminated the work of his predecessors with a grammatical description of Sanskrit which has been acclaimed as being the most detailed and comprehensive grammar ever done. The Indians were more interested in an accurate accounting of Sanskrit rather than answering philosophical questions.
2 Work in the Nineteenth Century was based essentially on the classical languages (Latin and Greek) and the then-oldest-attested Indo-European language, Sanskrit. Hittite was identified as an older Indo-European language subsequent to the uncovering of Hittite inscriptions in the 1870's about 150 kilometers east of Ankara, Turkey. Hittite became extinct by 1200 B.C.
3 Mispronunciation or changed pronunciations were caused by convenience not to be confused with laziness, neglect, or some natural ease of pronunciation of individual sounds. Instead, mispronunciation is an effect involving the assimilation of sounds and the influence of the symmetry (pattern pressure) of the entire sound system. These changes were not considered to be deteriorative in the sense of 'corrupting' language as linguists of earlier periods had suggested.
#9 Daily Journal
Tuesday is better that last day
I woke up at 3:00 a.m to do my homework because i over slept quickly. I also ate some foods because i did not take dinner. Then i went to campus alone because my friend was sick. After arrived, i continued doing my homework. But suddenly my chairman said that my lecturer for structure would not come. All of my friends were happy because we did not finish our homework yet. So far, this day was not really bad
I woke up at 3:00 a.m to do my homework because i over slept quickly. I also ate some foods because i did not take dinner. Then i went to campus alone because my friend was sick. After arrived, i continued doing my homework. But suddenly my chairman said that my lecturer for structure would not come. All of my friends were happy because we did not finish our homework yet. So far, this day was not really bad
#8 Daily Journal
Monday,
Last night, i drank a cup of coffee to make my mind staying with doing my home work and reading a book because there was a test. In fact, nothing...I felt, i was so unlucky. Sometimes i got angry, but i did not know why and for whom.
Last night, i drank a cup of coffee to make my mind staying with doing my home work and reading a book because there was a test. In fact, nothing...I felt, i was so unlucky. Sometimes i got angry, but i did not know why and for whom.
#7 Daily Journal
Nice Sunday ^_^
i woke up too late this morning..luckily, i could do my prefer. Then i did my usual activities like take a bath, have breakfast, and etc. I remembered, i had promised to my friend to do our power point. Then i went home to continue my homework. By the way, this day is not bad.
i woke up too late this morning..luckily, i could do my prefer. Then i did my usual activities like take a bath, have breakfast, and etc. I remembered, i had promised to my friend to do our power point. Then i went home to continue my homework. By the way, this day is not bad.
#6 Daily Journal
Saturday,
i just felt lonely because no one in my boarding house.
but i did some activities like edit my photos and did my homework.
Nothing special...
i just felt lonely because no one in my boarding house.
but i did some activities like edit my photos and did my homework.
Nothing special...
#5 Daily Journal
Pretty Friday
This day, i just had a class, it was SEA. In that class, we discussed what story that we will perform. We decided not to perform drama classical like senior's performance. We just choose between comedy or musical drama. Both of them are my favorite kinds of drama. Okay, i can not continue this because i am so hungry and i would like to buy something that make me full. See You......
This day, i just had a class, it was SEA. In that class, we discussed what story that we will perform. We decided not to perform drama classical like senior's performance. We just choose between comedy or musical drama. Both of them are my favorite kinds of drama. Okay, i can not continue this because i am so hungry and i would like to buy something that make me full. See You......
Phonetic
Phonetics (from the Greek: φωνή, phōnē, "sound, voice") is a branch of linguistics that comprises the study of the sounds of human speech, or—in the case of sign languages—the equivalent aspects of sign.[1] It is concerned with the physical properties of speech sounds or signs (phones): their physiological production, acoustic properties, auditory perception, and neurophysiological status. Phonology, on the other hand, is concerned with the abstract, grammatical characterization of systems of sounds or signs.
The field of phonetics is a multiple layered subject of linguistics that focuses on speech. In the case of oral languages there are three basic areas of study:
- Articulatory phonetics: the study of the production of speech sounds by the articulatory and vocal tract by the speaker
- Acoustic phonetics: the study of the physical transmission of speech sounds from the speaker to the listener
- Auditory phonetics: the study of the reception and perception of speech sounds by the listener
History
Phonetics was studied as early as 500 BC in ancient India, with Pāṇini's account of the place and manner of articulation of consonants in his 5th century BC treatise on Sanskrit. The major Indic alphabets today order their consonants according to Pāṇini's classification. The Ancient Greeks are credited as the first to base a writing system on a phonetic alphabet. Modern phonetics began with Alexander Melville Bell, whose Visible Speech (1867) introduced a system of precise notation for writing down speech sounds.[2]
Phonetic transcription
The International Phonetic Alphabet(IPA)
is used as the basis for the phonetic transcription of speech. It is
based on the Latin alphabet and is able to transcribe most features of
speech such as consonants, vowels, and suprasegmental features. Every
documented phoneme available within the known languages in the world is
assigned its own corresponding symbol.
The difference between phonetics and phonology
Phonology concerns itself with systems of phonem, abstract cognitive
units of speech sound or sign which distinguish the words of a
language. Phonetics, on the other hand, concerns itself with the
production, transmission, and perception of the physical phenomena which are abstracted in the mind to constitute these speech sounds or signs.
Using an Edison phonograph, Ludimar Hermann investigated the spectral properties of vowels and consonants. It was in these papers that the term formant
was first introduced. Hermann also played back vowel recordings made
with the Edison phonograph at different speeds in order to test Willis' and Wheatstone's theories of vowel production.
Relation to phonology
In contrast to phonetics, phonology
is the study of how sounds and gestures pattern in and across
languages, relating such concerns with other levels and aspects of
language. Phonetics deals with the articulatory and acoustic properties
of speech sounds, how they are produced, and how they are perceived. As
part of this investigation, phoneticians may concern themselves with the
physical properties of meaningful sound contrasts or the social meaning
encoded in the speech signal (e.g. gender, sexuality, ethnicity,
etc.). However, a substantial portion of research in phonetics is not
concerned with the meaningful elements in the speech signal.
While it is widely agreed that phonology is grounded in phonetics,
phonology is a distinct branch of linguistics, concerned with sounds and
gestures as abstract units (e.g., features, phonemes, mora, syllables, etc.) and their conditioned variation (via, e.g., allophonic rules, constraints, or derivational rules).[3] Phonology relates to phonetics via the set of distinctive features,
which map the abstract representations of speech units to articulatory
gestures, acoustic signals, and/or perceptual representations.[4][5][6]
Subfields
Phonetics as a research discipline has three main branches:
- articulatory phonetics is concerned with the articulation of speech: The position, shape, and movement of articulators or speech organs, such as the lips, tongue, and vocal folds.
- acoustic phonetics is concerned with acoustics of speech: The spectro-temporal properties of the sound waves produced by speech, such as their frequency, amplitude, and harmonic structure.
- auditory phonetics is concerned with speech perception: the perception, categorization, and recognition of speech sounds and the role of the auditory system and the brain in the same.
Types of notational systems
Most phonetic transcription is based on the assumption that
linguistic sounds are segmentable into discrete units that can be
represented by symbols.
This section requires expansion. |
Alphabetic
IPA
The International Phonetic Alphabet
(IPA) is one of the most popular and well-known phonetic alphabets. It
was originally created by primarily British language teachers, with
later efforts from European phoneticians and linguists. It has changed
from its earlier intention as a tool of foreign language pedagogy to a
practical alphabet of linguists. It is currently becoming the most often
seen alphabet in the field of phonetics.
Most American dictionaries for native English-speakers—American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, Random House Dictionary of the English Language, Webster's Third New International Dictionary—employ respelling systems based on the English alphabet, with diacritical marks over the vowels and stress marks.[1] (See Wikipedia:United States dictionary transcription for a generic version.)
Another commonly encountered alphabetic tradition was originally created for the transcription of Native American and European languages, and is still commonly used by linguists of Slavic, Indic, Uralic, Semitic, and Caucasian languages. This is sometimes labeled the Americanist
phonetic alphabet, but this is misleading because it has always been
widely used for languages outside the Americas. The difference between
these alphabets and IPA is small, although often the specially created
characters of the IPA are often abandoned in favour of already existing
characters with diacritics (e.g. many characters are borrowed from
Eastern European orthographies) or digraphs.
There are also extended versions of the IPA, for example: extIPA, VoQs, and Luciano Canepari's canIPA.
Aspects of alphabetic transcription
Other alphabets, such as Hangul, may have their own phonetic extensions. There also exist featural phonetic transcription systems, such as Alexander Melville Bell's Visible Speech and its derivatives.
The International Phonetic Association recommends that a phonetic transcription should be enclosed in square brackets "[ ]". A transcription that specifically denotes only phonological contrasts may be enclosed in slashes
"/ /" instead. If one is in doubt, it is best to use brackets, for by
setting off a transcription with slashes one makes a theoretical claim
that every symbol within is phonemically contrastive for the language being transcribed.
Phonetic transcriptions try to objectively capture the actual
pronunciation of a word, whereas phonemic transcriptions are
model-dependent. For example, in The Sound Pattern of English, Noam Chomsky and Morris Halle transcribed the English word night
phonemically as /nixt/. In this model, the phoneme /x/ is never
realized as [x], but shows its presence by "lengthening" the preceding
vowel. The preceding vowel in this case is the phoneme /i/, which is
pronounced [aɪ] when "long". So phonemic /nixt/ is equivalent to
phonetic [naɪt], but underlying this analysis is the belief that
historical sounds such as the gh in night may remain in a
word long after they have ceased to be pronounced, or that a phoneme may
exist in a language without ever being directly expressed. (This was
later rejected by both Chomsky and Halle.)
For phonetic transcriptions, there is flexibility in how closely
sounds may be transcribed. A transcription that gives only a basic idea
of the sounds of a language in the broadest terms is called a broad transcription;
in some cases this may be equivalent to a phonemic transcription (only
without any theoretical claims). A close transcription, indicating
precise details of the sounds, is called a narrow transcription. These are not binary choices, but the ends of a continuum, with many possibilities in between. All are enclosed in brackets.
For example, in some dialects the English word pretzel in a
narrow transcription would be [ˈpʰɹ̥ʷɛʔt.sɫ̩], which notes several
phonetic features that may not be evident even to a native speaker. An
example of a broad transcription is [ˈpʰɹɛt.sɫ̩], which only indicates
some of the easier to hear features. A yet broader transcription would
be [ˈpɹɛt.sl]. Here every symbol represents an unambiguous speech sound,
but without going into any unnecessary detail. None of these
transcriptions make any claims about the phonemic status of the sounds.
Instead, they represent certain ways in which it is possible to produce
the sounds that make up the word.
There are also several possibilities in how to transcribe this word
phonemically, but here the differences are generally not of precision,
but of analysis. For example, pretzel could be /ˈprɛt.sl̩/ or /ˈpret.səl/. The special symbol for English r is not used, for it is not meaningful to distinguish it from a rolled r. The differences in the letter e reflect claims as to what the essential difference is between the vowels of pretzel and pray;
there are half a dozen ideas in the literature as to what this may be.
The second transcription claims that there are two vowels in the word,
even if they can't both be heard, while the first claims there is only
one.
However, phonemic transcriptions may also be broad or narrow, or
perhaps it would be better to say abstract vs. concrete. They may show a
fair amount of phonetic detail, usually of a phoneme's most common
allophone, but because they are abstract symbols they do not need to
resemble any sound at all directly. Phonemic symbols will frequently be
chosen to avoid diacritics as much as possible, under a 'one sound one
symbol' policy, or may even be restricted to the ASCII symbols of a typical keyboard. For example, the English word church
may be transcribed as /t͡ʃɝːt͡ʃ/, a close approximation of its actual
pronunciation, or more abstractly as /crc/, which is easier to type.
Phonemic symbols should always be explained, especially when they are as
divergent from actual /crc/.
Occasionally a transcription will be enclosed in pipes ("| |"). This goes beyond phonology into morphological analysis. For example, the words pets and beds
could be transcribed phonetically as [pʰɛʔts] and [b̥ɛdz] (in a fairly
narrow transcription), and phonemically as /pets/ and /bedz/. Because
/s/ and /z/ are separate phonemes
in English, they receive separate symbols in the phonemic analysis.
However, you probably recognize that underneath this, they represent the
same plural ending. This can be indicated with the pipe notation. If
you believe the plural ending is essentially an s, as English
spelling would suggest, the words can be transcribed |pets| and |beds|.
If, as most linguists would probably suggest, it is essentially a z, these would be |petz| and |bedz|.
To avoid confusion with IPA symbols, it may be desirable to specify
when native orthography is being used, so that, for example, the English
word jet is not read as "yet". This is done with angle brackets or chevrons:
⟨jet⟩. It is also common to italicize such words, but the chevrons
indicate specifically that they are in the original language's
orthography, and not in English transliteration.
Iconic
Visible Speech
In iconic phonetic notation, the shapes of the phonetic
characters are designed so that they visually represent the position of
articulators in the vocal tract. This is unlike alphabetic notation,
where the correspondence between character shape and articulator
position is arbitrary. This notation is potentially more flexible than
alphabetic notation in showing more shades of pronunciation (MacMahon
1996:838–841). An example of iconic phonetic notation is the Visible Speech system, created by Scottish phonetician, Alexander Melville Bell (Ellis 1869:15).
Analphabetic
Another type of phonetic notation that is more precise than alphabetic notation is analphabetic
phonetic notation. Instead of both the alphabetic and iconic notational
types' general principle of using one symbol per sound, analphabetic
notation uses long sequences of symbols to precisely describe the
component features of an articulatory gesture (MacMahon 1996:842–844).
This type of notation is reminiscent of the notation used in chemical formulas
to denote the composition of chemical compounds. Although more
descriptive than alphabetic notation, analphabetic notation is less
practical for many purposes (e.g. for descriptive linguists doing
fieldwork or for speech pathologists impressionistically transcribing
speech disorders). As a result, this type of notation is uncommon.
Two examples of this type were developed by the Danish Otto Jespersen (1889) and American Kenneth Pike
(1943). Pike's system, which is part of a larger goal of scientific
description of phonetics, is particularly interesting in its challenge
against the descriptional method of the phoneticians who created
alphabetic systems like the IPA. An example of Pike's system can be
demonstrated by the following. A syllabic voiced alveolar nasal consonant (/n̩/ in IPA) is notated as
- MaIlDeCVoeIpvnnAPpaatdtltnransnsfSpvavdtlvtnransssfTpgagdtlwvtitvransnsfSrpFSs
In Pike's notation there are 5 main components (which are indicated using the example above):
- M - manner of production (i.e., MaIlDe)
- C - manner of controlling (i.e., CVoeIpvnn)
- description of stricture (i.e., what APpaatdtltnransnsfSpvavdtlvtnransssfTpgagdtlwvtitvransnsf)
- S - segment type (i.e., Srp)
- F - phonetic function (i.e., FSs)
The components of the notational hierarchy of this consonant are explained below:
|
|
|
#4 Daily Journal
How unlucky i am :(
i started today with praying for My Allah. I hoped this day was easy to pass especially in campus. On the other hand, that was just a wish...not being real. Maybe Allah wanted to me being more diligent and careful to finish all of my home work. Although i got it, i never forgot saying Thanks For Allah, i learned this day with a little smile
i started today with praying for My Allah. I hoped this day was easy to pass especially in campus. On the other hand, that was just a wish...not being real. Maybe Allah wanted to me being more diligent and careful to finish all of my home work. Although i got it, i never forgot saying Thanks For Allah, i learned this day with a little smile
Syntax
The syntax of a language is the set of rules that language uses to combine words and morphemes to create sentences. For example, in some languages adjectives come before the noun, and in others after the noun. Some languages use prepositions, others use postpositions. In some, the verb is at the beginning of the sentence, in others it is at the end, and in others still it is somewhere in the middle. But rules such as these only scrape the surface!
We start with a basic classification of words, the parts of speech (aka lexical categories): Nouns, verbs, adjectives, adverbs, prepositions, and so forth. These then combine into phrases.
A noun phrase consists of a noun and all its modifiers - i.e. determinants, adjectives, and any clauses or prepositional phrases that modify the noun. (We will get to clauses in a bit.) Traditionally, the noun is called the head of the noun phrase, because it determines the syntactic function of the phrase - that is, the phrase acts as if it were a noun. Examples: "Bob", "the cat in the hat", "a whole other problem", "the man who came to dinner", etc.
Some linguists use the term argument to refer to noun phrases, especially to the subject and the object in a sentence.
A verb phrase consists of a verb and all its modifiers - i.e. adverbs, auxiliary verbs, prepositional phrases, and adverbial clauses. Most linguists would also include noun phrases such as the object and complements, in the definition of a verb phrase. The verb is, of course, the head of the verb phrase. Examples: "stop!", "walk carefully", "will soon greatly regret", "came in the middle of the night", "hurt his pride", "put out the cat", etc.
Just to make things more complicated, some linguists use the term predicate, which overlaps with "verb phrase".
A prepositional phrase consists of a preposition (which is the head of the prepositional phrase, of course) and a noun phrase. If the language uses postpositions, we call it a postpositional phrase. Together, they are called adpositional phrases.
Finally, there are adjectival phrases and adverbial phrases. If you say "that man is full of bull", "full of bull" (an adjective "full" followed by a prepositional phrase "of bull") is an adjective phrase. Similarly, in "he walked very slowly", "very slowly" (an adverb "slowly" preceded by a modifier "very") is an adverbial phrase.
Complements are words or phrases (and sometimes even clauses) that tell us more about a noun or a noun phrase, but do so by means of a verb. For example, in the sentence "he looks sick", "sick" modifies "he", but uses the verb "looks" to make the connection. In "we painted the town red", "red" modifies "town" by means of the verb "painted". In "I am in a good mood", "in a good mood" modifies "I", by means of the verb "am".
As you can see by the examples, the most common verb used to link a complement to a noun is the verb "to be" - known in linguistics as a copula. Other copulas include "become", "seems", "turned", "appears", etc. The copula with a noun complement ("he is the king") is sometimes called a predicate nominal. The copula with an adjective ("he is stupid") is sometimes called a predicate adjective.
Clauses
A clause is a set of words that includes at least a verb and probably a subject noun. In some cases and languages, the subject may be implied. A sentence is actually a clause. But a sentence can have more than one clause: There may be a main clause (or independent clause) and one or more subordinate clauses. This kind of sentence is called a complex sentence.
Subordinate clauses come in several forms:
An adverbial clause is one which modifies the main clause in the manner of an adverb. They are typically introduced by subordinating conjunctions. For example:
I am not afraid of the dog, because
it is very small.
If they lose weight too quickly, they will regain it afterwards.
I'm running so that the rhinos don't catch me.
He wrote when his mother asked.
He is happy where he lives.
I wasn't allowed to do things as I wanted.
If they lose weight too quickly, they will regain it afterwards.
I'm running so that the rhinos don't catch me.
He wrote when his mother asked.
He is happy where he lives.
I wasn't allowed to do things as I wanted.
An adjectival clause (or relative clause) is one that modifies a noun phrase. They typically are introduced by a relative pronoun. For example:
The man who
lived here went to New York.
The car that came from Italy is very fast.
The apple which fell from my bag is now inedible.
The car that came from Italy is very fast.
The apple which fell from my bag is now inedible.
The noun phrase can also be the object of the adjectival clause, or even the object of a preposition. Notice how, in English, we often drop the relative pronoun:
The woman (whom)
I love comes from France.
The robot (that) I built doesn't work.
Let's go to my house, which is nearby.
The robot (that) I built doesn't work.
Let's go to my house, which is nearby.
The woman, of whom
we speak, works at my office.
Your book, in which I wrote her name, is on the table.
Your book, in which I wrote her name, is on the table.
Finally, adjectival clauses are sometimes introduced by a relative adverb. Notice how, if you drop the noun, the clause becomes an adverbial clause:
I eat in Paris, where
I live. (I eat where I live.)
He will visit in July, when the weather is good. (He will visit when the weather is good.)
That's the reason why Juan left. (That's the reason Juan left; That's why Juan left.)
He will visit in July, when the weather is good. (He will visit when the weather is good.)
That's the reason why Juan left. (That's the reason Juan left; That's why Juan left.)
A noun clause is one which plays the part of a noun in the larger sentence. They too are typically introduced by relative pronouns or adverbs. If you want to figure out if you are looking at a noun clause, substitute him, her, it, or them for relative word. If you still have a meaningful sentence, then the clause was a noun clause. Notice also that, if you were to add a noun before the relative pronoun or adverb, you change the clause into an adjectival clause:
I see where you are. (I see the
place where you are.)
When I will depart, I just don't know. (The time when I will depart, I just don't know.)
I know who said that. (I know the person who said that.)
I heard what you said. (I heard the thing you said.)
I can guess which he's reading. (I can guess which book he's reading.)
When I will depart, I just don't know. (The time when I will depart, I just don't know.)
I know who said that. (I know the person who said that.)
I heard what you said. (I heard the thing you said.)
I can guess which he's reading. (I can guess which book he's reading.)
There are also noun clauses that are introduced by a subordinating conjunction such as the English "that":
I know that
you don't like me.
She thinks (that) I'm silly.
They were surprised that he could jump that high.
She thinks (that) I'm silly.
They were surprised that he could jump that high.
Finally, a sentence can have two or more main (independent) clauses, joined by coordinating conjuntions. This kind of sentence is called a compound sentence:
I want to sing and I want to dance, but I have no talent.
Either he goes or I go.
Either he goes or I go.
Kinds of sentences
Most sentences are simple statements about the world or one's thoughts or feelings. These are called declarative sentences or just declarations. Most of the examples above a declarations.
Questions are a kind of sentence that often involves complications of syntax in many languages. First, there are three kinds of questions. The first is the yes/no question. In these examples, you can see some of the syntactical techniques we use in English, such adding "do", moving the verb, or adding a tag such as "did you?"
Are you going to town?
Do they like it?
You didn't forget, did you?
Do they like it?
You didn't forget, did you?
There are also choice questions, which require a simple answer selected from the options given in the question:
Do you want broccoli or cauliflower?
Should I wear my jacket or a sweater?
Should I wear my jacket or a sweater?
The final kind is the wh- question, which involves the use of interrogative words (pronouns and adverbs) such as who, what, which, when, where, how, how many, and why. These require a fuller answer:
Why
are you going to town?
How many are going with you?
And when do you leave?
How many are going with you?
And when do you leave?
Another kind of sentence is the command. The usual kind involves the speaker telling someone else to do something, such as "pick up your clothes!". Commands may also involve exhortations to one's own group, such as in the sentence "let's go!" And commands can also blend into the question category of sentences when we make polite requests, such as "would you pass the tea, please?"
One common syntactic "trick" for commands is to drop the subject, which is understood. In English, most commands are directed at "you", so why bother including that pronoun? If the command is directed at "us", then we use "let us" before the verb. Other languages have other techniques, of course.
The last kind of sentence is the exclamation. This kind of sentence expresses an emotional reaction to the situation. For example, "what a lovely day!" tells us not only that it's a lovely day, but that I am exceedingly pleased by it. Notice that this particular sentence does not contain a verb, and that is quite common in exclamations. In fact, many exclamations are single words: "crap!" "oy!" "weeee!" "hello!" etc. These are called word-sentences, and the words are called sentence words.
Phrase structure
Traditionally, linguists have several ways of diagramming the structure of a sentence. One way is called phrase structure rules. For example:
S -> NP VP
"A sentence is made up of a noun phrase
and a verb phrase."
NP -> (Det) (AP) N (PP)
"A noun phrase is composed of a
noun plus optional determinantes, adjective phrases, and prepositional
phrases."
VP -> (Aux) V (NP) (PP) (AdvP)
"A verb phrase is composed of a verb
plus optional auxiliary verbs, object noun phrases, prepositional
phrases, and adverbial phrases."
AP -> (AdvP) A
"An adjective phrase is composed of an
adjective and optional adverbial phrases."
PP -> Prep NP
"A prepositional phrase is composed of
a preposition and a noun phrase."
AdvP -> (Adv) Adv
"An adverbial phrase is composed of an
adverb and optional modifying adverbs."
These phrase structure rules can then be used to construct phrase trees. Here's one of the most famous of these trees, concerning the famous linguist Chomsky's famous meaningless (but grammatical!) sentence "Colorless green ideas sleep furiously":
In Chomsky's original theory, each
language has a basic syntax - set of rules for the construction of
basic sentences - and another set of rules for altering those basic
sentences to fit particular uses. That second set of rules are called transformation rules, and they look
a lot like the phrase structure rules we just discussed. For
example, in English, we can take a basic sentence like "He likes
apples" and turn it into the question "does he like apples?" via
certain rules. In English, we would take the auxiliary verb and move it
to the beginning of the sentence. This is called subject-auxiliary reversal. If there
is no auxiliary verb, we have to insert a "dummy" verb "do" and move
that to the beginning of the sentence:
To turn that same sentence into "what does he like?" we start with "he likes X". We perform the subject-auxiliary reversal to turn it into a question ("he does like X" followed by "does he like X?"). We substitute the interrogative pronoun "what" for X, then move it to the front of the sentence ("does he like what?" followed by "what does he like?"). This last step is called wh- movement:
N1 V N2 => N1 Aux V N2
"Insert the auxiliary do in front of the verb."
N1 Aux V N2 => Aux N1 V N2
"Move the auxiliary do to the front of the
sentence."
To turn that same sentence into "what does he like?" we start with "he likes X". We perform the subject-auxiliary reversal to turn it into a question ("he does like X" followed by "does he like X?"). We substitute the interrogative pronoun "what" for X, then move it to the front of the sentence ("does he like what?" followed by "what does he like?"). This last step is called wh- movement:
N1 V N2 => N1 Aux V N2
"Add the auxiliary do in front of the verb."
N1 Aux V N2 => Aux N1 V N2
"Move the auxiliary do to the front of the sentence."
Aux N1 V N2 > Aux N1 V Pn
"Place the interrogative pronoun what where the answer would
be."
Aux N1 V Pn > Pn Aux N1 V
"Move the interrogative pronoun what to the front of the
sentence."
For obvious reasons, this theory was called transformational grammar. Since Chomsky, theories of syntax have been either (1) elaborations of transformational grammar or (2) alternatives to transformational grammar.
One interesting offshoot of Chomsky's original theories is the idea of universal grammar. This is the idea that all language begins with a single deep structure, the structure from which the transformations proceed. Furthermore, that deep structure is innate to the human brain in the form of a language acquisition device or LAD. The fact that other intelligent animals, such as the great apes, whales, dolphins, and elephants don't have anything close to the language capabilities of humans is said to be because they lack this evolutionary development. On the other hand, nothing quite as fully organized as the hypothetical LAD has been found in the human brain.
One family of alternative theories is called construction grammar (or CxG), which is strongly linked to developments in cognitive and cognitive developmental psychology. The basic idea here is that each language has a number of constructions a bit like Piaget's schemas. In fact, it postulates a whole taxonomy of these constructions - one for yes-no questions, one for simple commands, one for talking about how you feel about something, etc. etc. etc. So, instead of learning a list of transformations, we learn a list of constructions. At the same time, when you learn a word, you also learn the kinds of constructions that word can fit in, in very much the same way that you learn implicitly that "cat" is a noun which therefore can be modified by adjectives and be the subject or object of a verb, and so on. As a psychologist, I find construction grammar more appealing than transformational grammar.
Morphology
Morphology is the study of
morphemes, obviously. Morphemes
are words, word stems, and affixes, basically the unit of language one
up from phonemes. Although they are often understood as units of
meaning, they are usually considered a part of a language's syntax or
grammar. It is specifically grammatical morphemes that this
chapter will focus on.
It is in their morphology that we most clearly see the differences between languages that are isolating (such as Chinese, Indonesian, Krewol...), ones that are agglutinating (such as Turkish, Finnish, Tamil...), and ones that are inflexional (such as Russian, Latin, Arabic...). Isolating languages use grammatical morphemes that are separate words. Agglutinating languages use grammatical morphemes in the form of attached syllables called affixes. Inflexional languages may go one step further and actually change the word at the phonemic level to express grammatical morphemes.
All languages are really mixed systems -- it's all a matter of proportions. English, for example, uses all three methods: To make the future tense of a verb, we use the particle will (I will see you); to make the past tense, we usually use the affix -ed (I changed it); but in many words, we change the word for the past (I see it becomes I saw it). Looking at nouns, sometimes we make the plural with a particle (three head of cattle), sometimes with an affix (three cats), and sometimes by changing the word (three men). But, because we still use a lot of non-syllable affixes (such as -ed, usually pronounced as d or t, and -s, usually pronounced as s or z, dependeing on context), English is still considered an inflexional language by most linguists.
It is in their morphology that we most clearly see the differences between languages that are isolating (such as Chinese, Indonesian, Krewol...), ones that are agglutinating (such as Turkish, Finnish, Tamil...), and ones that are inflexional (such as Russian, Latin, Arabic...). Isolating languages use grammatical morphemes that are separate words. Agglutinating languages use grammatical morphemes in the form of attached syllables called affixes. Inflexional languages may go one step further and actually change the word at the phonemic level to express grammatical morphemes.
All languages are really mixed systems -- it's all a matter of proportions. English, for example, uses all three methods: To make the future tense of a verb, we use the particle will (I will see you); to make the past tense, we usually use the affix -ed (I changed it); but in many words, we change the word for the past (I see it becomes I saw it). Looking at nouns, sometimes we make the plural with a particle (three head of cattle), sometimes with an affix (three cats), and sometimes by changing the word (three men). But, because we still use a lot of non-syllable affixes (such as -ed, usually pronounced as d or t, and -s, usually pronounced as s or z, dependeing on context), English is still considered an inflexional language by most linguists.
Affixes
Most languages, but especially agglutinating and inflexional ones,
differentiate between the stem of the word, which carries the
basic meaning, and various affixes or attachments that carry
additional, often grammatical, meanings. There are several kinds
of affixes:
Suffixes are attached to the end of the stem;Suffixes are the most common, and English uses them. For example, the past tense of most verbs is a matter of adding -ed to the stem; the present participle is made by adding -ing; the plural of a noun is made by adding -s.
Prefixes are attached to the front of the stem;
Infixes are put in the middle of the word;
Ablaut is a change in a vowel that carries extra meaning;
Reduplication is a matter of doubling a syllable to do the same.
Turkish is an example of an agglutinating language that makes
extensive use of suffixes. One example I found on the internet
(Learning Practical Turkish) is the word terbiyesizliklerindenmis:
good manners | terbiye |
without good manners, rude | terbiyesiz |
rudeness | terbiyesizlik |
their rudeness | terbiyesizlikleri |
from their rudeness | terbiyesizliklerinden |
I gather that it was from their rudeness | terbiyesizliklerindenmis |
Note that a language doesn't necessarily need to be agglutinating to have long words. German, for example, has Rindfleischetikettierungsüberwachungsaufgabenübertragungsgesetz, and English has Pneumonoultramicroscopicsilicovolcanoconiosis!
Although prefix languages are a bit rarer, they can be every bit as expansive.
Ablaut is common in English and its Germanic cousins. For
example, the past tense of sing
is sang, and the past participle is sung. The
plural of
goose is geese. Ablaut seems to come from former
suffixes
that influenced the pronunciation of the vowel, then disappeared over
time.
Goose-geese was once gos-gese, and before that gos-göse,
and before that gos-gose. The plural suffix -e caused the
fronting
of the vowel o to ö and then e.
Infixes are best illustrated by the Semitic languages, such as
Arabic. Many words in Arabic are composed of three consonants,
and many of the grammatical
variations are produced by altering the vowels between and around
them.
For example, the root for writing is ktb:
to write | kataba |
writing | kaatib |
a book | kitaab |
books | kutub |
author | kattaab |
Perhaps, thousands of years ago, some people began generalized from ablauts -- as if we were to start saying pan-pen (rather than pan-pans), following the pattern of man-men.
Irish (and other Celtic languages, such as Gaelic, Welsh, and Breton) are unusual in that it is the consonants that change in various situations, rather than the vowels. Note that th is pronounced h, dt is pronounced d, ea is a, ch is as in loch, and bh is v.
house | flat | ||
teach | árasán | ||
my | mo | mo theach | m'árasán |
your | do | do theach | d'árasán |
his | a | a theach | a árasán |
her | a | a teach | a hárasán |
our | ar | ar dteach | ar n-árasán |
your | bhur | bhur dteach | bhur n-árasán |
their | a | a dteach | a n-árasán |
Nouns
Nouns are words that name or denote a person, thing, action,
or
quality. They are “thing” words -- although “things” can include
all
sorts of abstract ideas that might otherwise look more like verbs or
adjectives.
In various languages, they are marked, by affixes or particles, as to
their
number, gender, definiteness, and especially cases.
Definiteness concerns the extent to which we are talking
about a
specific thing or event, one that is known to the speakers, or about
something
less well defined, such as any old thing, or something not specific.
In English, the definite is marked by the article the.
It
can also be marked by other words, such as this, that, my, yours,
and
so on. The indefinite is marked by the article a or an,
as well as the plural without an article, or words such as one,
two, some,
any, etc. On the other hand, many languages don't use
articles at
all -- Latin, Russian, Hindi, and Chinese come to mind!
In a number of languages, the definite is marked with a
suffix. This
is true of Swedish, Danish, Norwegian, Icelandic, Rumanian, Bulgarian,
and
Albanian, among others. The Scandinavian languages are, of
course, closely related, so we would expect them to share a feature
like this. But Rumanian, Bulgarian, and Albanian are only
distantly related. It seems that they influenced each other, or perhaps
there were people living in the Balkans in ancient times who influenced
them all.
Number, of course, refers to how many of the item we are
talking about. There are three common numbers: Singular,
meaning
one; plural, meaning more than one; and somewhat rarer,
the dual, meaning two. You can see the significance of
the dual in our own use
of words such as couple, pair, and so on. Again, many
languages
do not mark the plural, much less the dual.
The most complex aspect of nouns is cases, also known as declensions.
Philosophers
in ancient Greece and India were already discussing this
as much
as 2500 years ago! Much of the terminology we still use today was
invented
during the Roman Empire, and reflects the cases used in Latin.
The first case is the nominative, roughly the subject of the
sentence.
In many languages, it is the basic form, sometimes represented by the
bare
stem. A second case is the vocative, which is the form
used
when calling out to someone, sort of like “Oh, Claudius!” The
rest
of the cases are referred to as oblique or objective.
Languages
that make many distinctions among the oblique cases use them
in
the same way that other languages use prepositions or postpositions.
There are many others. A language in the Caucasus Mountains called Tassaran has 48 noun cases! However, many linguists point out that cases should only refer to inflexional languages such as Latin. Agglutinative languages such as Finnish can be better thought of as having postpositions that are attached to the noun, since they are very consistent and easy to recognize, unlike the cases in Latin.
Accusative -- the direct object of the verb: He threw the ball. Dative -- the indirect object: He threw it to John. Ablative -- expressed in English with the preposition from: He threw from first base. Locative -- expressed in English with prepositions such as at or in: We were at the hotdog stand in the stadium. Genitive -- the possessive form, often expressed in English with the word of, but also with the case suffix ‘s: It was John’s ball. Instrumental -- expressed in English with prepositions like with: He hit it with a bat. Sociative -- also expressed in English with with, but now referring to people: I went out with her.
Here is an example of the Russian word for country, singular and plural:
singular | plural | |
nominative | strana | strany |
accusative | stranu | strany |
genitive | strany | stran |
dative | strane | stranam |
instrumental | stranoj | stranomi |
locative | strane | stranax |
This wouldn’t be such a strain, until you realize that there are several different declensions, and quite a few exceptions as well.
Compare that with an example of the word for man in Tamil, a Dravidian
language of southern India:
singular | plural | |
nominative | manitan | manitarkal |
accusative | manitanai | manitarkalai |
dative | manitanukku | manitarkalukku |
sociative | manitanotu | manitarkalotu |
genitive | manitanutaiya | manitarkalutaiya |
instrumental | manitanal | manitarkalal |
locative | manitanitam | manitarkalitam |
ablative | manitanitamiruntu | manitarkalitamiruntu |
Although there are even more cases, these endings are the same for
all
other
nouns! And notice how the plural is just a matter of sticking kal
inbetween the
stem and the affix.
One interesting side issue: In most languages, the subject of an
intransitive verb (he sits)
is in the same form (i.e. the nominative) as the subject of a
transitive verb (he sees him),
and
the object of a transitive verb is different (i.e. the
accusative). These languages are known as nominative-accusative
languages. But there are also languages where the subject of an
intransitive verb is in the same form as the object of a transitive
verb (i.e. the absolutive),
and the subject of a transitive verb is different (i.e. the ergative). In these languages, it
would be as if we said he sees him
but then him sits! These
are
called ergative-absolutive
languages.
Among the ergative-absolutive languages are Basque, the northern Caucasian languages, many Australian aborigine languages, Eskimo-Aleut, and many other languages of north and central America. They are all verb-first or verb-last languages.
Among the ergative-absolutive languages are Basque, the northern Caucasian languages, many Australian aborigine languages, Eskimo-Aleut, and many other languages of north and central America. They are all verb-first or verb-last languages.
Gender is perhaps the oddest noun variation. It is
called gender because it is -- loosely -- tied to the physical sex of
people and animals. Many languages differentiate between masculine
nouns and feminine nouns, with different endings for each, and
requiring different articles and adjective forms along with them.
French, Spanish, Italian, and Portuguese are examples.
Other languages, such as German, count three genders:
Masculine, feminine, and neuter. Neuter presumably refers
to things that don’t have a gender, but there is little consistency
there. In Dutch, there are two genders, but they are neuter and common,
common
deriving from what was originally masculine and feminine.
English nouns have no gender.
Many languages outside the European sphere differentiate between animate
and inanimate, one referring to people, animals, and spirits,
the other
to things. And there are many languages that make many
differentiations: Bantu languages, for example, have many noun
categories, such as "long, thin
things," "body parts," "places," and so on.
In Chinese, there is a strong isolating (non-affix) version of
this: When
you
want to indicate more than one of something, you must use a special
word
called a classifier between the number and the object.
This
is analogous to the way we might say three head of cattle.
There are still more examples of noun variation: Diminutives
express smallness (dog becomes doggy, for example), and
augmentatives express largeness. Diminutives are often
also used to express affection, and augmentatives sometimes express
danger or evil.
Some languages have a variety of honorifics, often suffixes
or prefixes
that indicate status. The Japanese -san is a well known
example.
There are also affixes that indicate lowly status, and in some
languages
several different degrees of status!
Pronouns
Pronouns are words that serve as place-holders for
nouns. Instead of referring to a person by his or her name, we
use he or she; instead of naming something repeatedly,
we refer to it as it. Pronouns have many of the same
variations as nouns, including gender, number, and case. There
are also three persons that are differentiated in most
languages: First refers to the person speaking or his/her
group (I, me; we, us); Second person refers to the person
spoken to
or his/her group (you); And the third person refers to other
people
outside
the conversation or to things (he, him, she, her, it, they, them).
In
English, for example...
nominative | oblique | possessive (adjective) |
possessive (pronomial) |
|
first person singular | I | me | my | mine |
second person singular | you | your | yours | |
third person singular male | he | him | his | |
third person singular female | she | her | hers | |
third person singular neuter | it | its | ||
first person plural | we | us | our | ours |
second person plural | you | your | yours | |
third person plural | they | them | their | theirs |
("Oblique" is the name for a case that covers the objects of a verb
or any preposition.)
For comparison, here's the Icelandic declension of the pronoun anyone:
masculine | feminine | neuter | ||
singular | ||||
nominative | nokkur | nokkur | nokkurt | |
accusative | nokkurn | nokkra | nokkurt | |
dative | nokkrum | nokkurri | nokkru | |
gentive | nokkurs | nokkurrar | nokkurs | |
plural | ||||
nominative | nokkrir | nokkrar | nokkur | |
accusative | nokkra | nokkrar | nokkur | |
dative | nokkrum | nokkrum | nokkrum | |
genitive | nokkurra | nokkura | nokkura |
In some languages, there are two forms of the third person
plural: One is inclusive, and refers to the speaker and
the listener together (Why don’t we go have a drink together
sometime?); the other is exclusive, and refers to the
speaker’s group distinct from the listener (We are going
to beat your team!).
There are also pronouns that reflect the action back onto the
subject --
appropriately named reflexive pronouns. In English, they
are
often nicely marked with -self (myself, yourself, himself,
etc.).
In many languages, there is a generic reflexive for the third person
singular
or even third person singular and plural. In Spanish, for
example,
that function is performed by the single word se.
Politeness is often an issue with pronouns. In many European
languages, there is a distinction made between a familiar and a
formal version of the second person singular. In French,
for example, you call
your friends tu and your parents, teachers, or boss vous.
You
don’t switch to tu until it is subtly agreed between the
two
of
you that it is okay to “tutoyer.” In some Asian
languages, there
is considerably more detail involved.
There are other kinds of pronouns besides the personal ones. Demonstrative
pronouns include this, that, these, and those.
Many languages
have three sets of these, one for things nearby the speaker, one for
things
nearby the listener, and one for things away from either.
Indefinite pronouns include words such as someone,
anyone, many,
and so on. Like the indefinite article, they don’t indicate
precisely
whom are what we are talking about.
Interrogative pronouns are used to ask question: Who
is
that
man? Relative pronouns are used to connect a
noun with
a clause that gives more detail about the noun: He is the one
whom
you saw yesterday. As you can see, in English, these two
groups of pronouns are often
the
same!
Verbs
Verbs are words which express action taken by something, the
state
something is in or a change in that state, or an interaction between
one
thing and another. Like nouns, there are many variations of
verbs.
Transitive verbs are ones that have both a subject and an
object: John hit the ball. John is the subject and ball
the object of
the verb hit. Intransitive verbs are ones that only have
a subject: I laughed. There is nothing that is laughed
(except, I suppose, the laugh itself.) Many verbs have an
intermediate form called the reflexive, meaning that the
subject is also the object: I hurt myself. As the
example shows, reflexive verb forms often take a reflexive pronoun as
their object! But there are reflexive verbs that don’t: They
got married.
The biggest issue with verb forms is conjugation. In
some languages, it is a fairly simple matter; in others, there are a
huge variety of affixes.
Most familiar to Europeans are tenses. Many languages
differentiate between the past tense, the present
tense, and the future tense. Some languages also
differentiate various details of timing, such as an immediate
form, a proximate form (near in time), and a distal
form (the distant past or future.) Quite a few languages
(Russian and Japanese included) only distingish past from "non-past."
In French, I sing goes through the following changes:
past (definite) | chantai |
present | chante |
future | chanterai |
Aspect is actually much older, and seems to tie into our psychology as human beings. The perfect aspect (as well as the similar completive or aorist) tells us that the action is finished, completed, “perfected.” In English, it is represented by various forms of the word to have, followed by the past participle: I had said (past perfect, aka pluperfect), I have said (present perfect), I will have said (future perfect). As the last one suggests, by the time we reach a particular point in the future, my saying something will be over and done with.
There is a passive version of the perfect called the effective.
In
English, an example might be He got seen.
The imperfect (aka durative or continuative)
has an
ongoing tone to it: The action continues through the
moment. In
English, we use a form of the verb to be followed by the
present participle:
I was saying, I am saying, I will be saying.
There are a number of variations on the imperfect aspect. The progressive
-- I have been saying -- suggests that the action started a bit
earlier
and continues through the present. The iterative (aka repetitive)
--
I keep saying -- indicates that a single action is
repeatedly performed.
And the inceptive (aka commencement) -- Let’s get
going
-- says to us that the action should get started.
Finally, there is the simple (or indefinite)
aspect. This includes the usual tenses used as is: I
said, I say, I will say. The simple past is often called the preterite.
Next up is mood or mode. The basic form is the indicative:
We
are saying something that happened, is happening, or will
happen. A version of the indicative is the stative, which
indicates that someone
or something is in a particular state, as opposed to taking a
particular action:
He sits.
The next three are used when there is a degree of unreality
involved, and
are often blended together. The optative (aka desiderative)
indicates
a desire or wish for something to happen. In English,
this is usually expressed with auxiliary (helper) verbs such as should
or
would, as well as with expressions such as I wish....
The conditional mood is used when the reality of one event
depends on the reality of another: I will go if you go.
English
has the remnants of a conditional: We say If I were
to go... rather than If I was
to go.... But it is
rapidly going the way
of the who-whom distinction!
The subjunctive mood is used when there is some doubt or
uncertainty about the event. Many languages have entire
conjugations of subjunctive, in various tenses and aspects. It
was the bane of my high school French
class.
There are other moods. In Japanese, for example, there are provisional
and tentative versions of verbs. And many languages have
the imperative: Do this! In English this is
expressed by
leaving out the subject (you).
In French, the aspect and mood variations on I sing look like this:
imperfect | chantais |
conditional | chanterais |
present subjunctive | chante |
Next, we have various voices.
The
active voice is
the basic one. It is used when the subject performs an action.
The passive voice is used when the subject of the sentence
is actually
the object of the action. In English, we use a form of to be
with the past participle: I was hit.
The causative is a voice used when the subject causes the
object to perform an action, as in He made me do it.
When the causative is combined with the reflexive, it is called the dynamic:
They married themselves!
Person is an aspect of verb forms in many languages.
Most commonly, there is an ending or other affix that indicates
something about the subject (such as first, second, or third person,
gender, and singular or plural). In English, the only person
ending left in almost all verbs
is the -s in the third person singular of the present tense (he does,
vs I, you, we, he, she, it, or they do).
There are languages (Basque comes to mind) where the direct object
and even the indirect object is also included in the verb form. Dakarzkizu,
for
example, means he brings them to you, while Zenekarzkidan
means you brought them to me. (Kar is the piece of these words
that is the equivalent to bring in English)
Here's a simple French conjugation, in the present tense, showing
person:
singular | plural | |
first person | chante | chantons |
second person | chantes | chantez |
third person | chante | chantent |
In addition, some languages have variations that express various levels of politeness. In Japanese, for example, Hon o katta means I bought a book -- but in a sort of abrupt, no nonsense way. Hon o kaimashita means I bought the book, but more politely expressed.
Another common verb variation is the negative. In
English, we use the word not after one of several auxiliary
(see below) verbs. There is a tendency, however, for many verbs
to change in the negative, by combining with the not: I
can’t, I won’t, I don’t, I ain’t.... Although we can still
see
where they come from (and the apostrophe reminds us), they are
well
on there way to becoming separate forms.
There are other languages where the verb changes when it is a part
of a
question. In Irish, for example tá (to be)
becomes an
bhfuil in questions.
I can't move on without mentioning that in Hausa (a language of
Nigeria), tense, aspect, etc., are indicated with variations of the
subject pronoun, not the verb, as in these example of the word for he:
perfect | kin |
future | záaki |
predictive | kyâa |
habitual | kíkàn |
subjunctive | kì |
continuous | kínàa |
(It might surprise you to know that we are moving this way in
English,
too: I'd, I've, I'll, etc.)
In isolating languages such as Chinese, or in languages moving strongly
in
that direction, such as English and French, many of the preceding
variations are not done by adding endings or changing the verb.
They are done with
auxiliary verbs. In English, for example, we say He
will
sing, rather than Il chantera as in French. In
French, on
the other hand, we often say Il a chanté instead
of He
sang.
These particular examples are called compound tenses, but they
can
also involve aspects and moods and so on.
Participles are forms of the verb that are often used in such
compound
verbs. In English, we have two: The past participle
(which
usually ends in -ed, just like the past tense) and the present
participle
(which ends in -ing). Participles are also used as adjectives: He
is
a dancing fool. He was a beaten man. And
they can
even be used as nouns: Help the down-trodden. Winning
is everything.
Note that the past participle is often referred to as the passive
participle, and the present participle as the active
participle.
Another form of the verb often used in compound verbs is the infinitive.
In
English, we don't have a real infinitive form -- we just put to
in front of it: To sleep, perchance to dream....
And so
we say He wants to run, a compound made with wants plus
the
infinitive of run. In many languages, there is a special
form.
In French, for example, it usually ends in -r, and is used as the
dictionary
form.
There are many forms of verbal nouns (gerunds) --
i.e. verbs
used as nouns, with or without special endings. The infinitive
and
the participles are examples. But we can also use the verb as is
in
many languages -- English being the best example, since we do it all
the
time: I dance and I go to the dance and I do
a dance and I devote my life to the dance!
Other Parts of Speech
Adjectives are words which modify nouns. In many
languages, adjectives have affixes that must agree with their nouns in
case, number, gender, etc.
One peculiar feature of adjectives in many language is comparison:
There
may be special forms of the adjective when you are using it to
say that
a noun is more or less of whatever quality the adjective expresses (the
comparative
form), or that is is the most or least of that quality (the superlative
form). In English, we still see special words like good/better/best,
regular endings such as big/bigger/biggest, and analytic forms
such
as significant/more significant/most significant.
Adverbs are words or phrases which modify verbs, adjectives,
or
even other adverbs. There are often special endings that
differentiate adverbs from similar adjectives: In English,
adverbs often end in -ly; In French, they often end in -ment.
Sometimes, adverbs are used to ask questions or to introduce certain
kinds of subordinate clauses which tell more about such things as when,
where, and how the action will happen. For example, when will
you be going? and I will go when I am good and ready.
Numerals (or just numbers) often come in both
adjective and
adverbial forms. In Shakespeare's time, we said three men, but
it was done thrice. Today, of course, the latter is analytic:
It was done three times.
The simple form of numerals is the cardinal number, which
indicates a certain quantity of something. There is also the ordinal
number,
which indicates the position of something in a sequence: He was the
third
man. We see a analytic construction more and more frequently
today:
He was her number one man, or she was bachelorette number
three.
Prepositions are words which can allow a noun to qualify
another noun or a verb in a way that parallels adjectives or
adverbs: The man in the yard ran into the
house. Many languages -- Japanese, for example -- have postpositions
instead of prepositions, but they serve the same purpose. Noun
cases are often a substitute for
prepositions or postpositions, and may in fact have developed out of
them.
Irish is interesting in that its prepositions ofen vary by person,
just
like verbs: Here is the "conjugation" of the preposition roimh
(before)
(mh is pronounced w):
before me | romham |
before you (singular) | romhat |
before him | roimhe |
before her | roimpi |
before us | romhainn |
before you (plural) | romhaibh |
before them | rompu |
Conjunctions are words that connect two parts of a sentence. There are two kinds of conjunctions. The most familiar are the coordinating conjunctions, such as and, or, and but. The second kind are the subordinating conjunctions (sometimes just called subordinators) such as if, because, so that, that, etc. These introduce certain kinds of subordinate clauses, such as I work so that I can feed my children and I think that she is lovely.
Finally, there are interjections. Interjections are
expressions of emotion -- not true words but rather vocal noises that
reflect the feelings of the speaker: Oh! Huh?
Hey! Shit! The
last one is, of course, also a regular word, but its use in this case
has
nothing to do with what it literally refers to.
Langganan:
Postingan (Atom)
Blog Subscription
Search this blog
Blogger templates
Popular Posts
-
Compounding Compounding forms a word out of two or more root morphemes. The words are called compounds or compound words. In Linguist...
-
Starting the day with a smile ^_^ I woke up at 5:30 a.m, it was late from my usual time at 5:00 a.m. My alarm was rang. I was not so aware ...
-
Morphology is the study of morphemes, obviously. Morphemes are words, word stems, and affixes, basically the unit of language one up fro...
-
Our examination of morphological processes has thus far concentrated on morphological structure and lexical rules. It is now time to consid...
-
Sociolinguistics is a quickly developing branch of linguistics which investigates the individual and social variation of language. Just a...
-
Pragmatics is a systematic way of explaining language use in context . It seeks to explain aspects of meaning which cannot be found in the...
-
Wednesday is a confusion day, "Hari Galau" -__-" I woke up caused i had a bad dream. In my dream, there was a man who asked...
-
Study of the nature and structure of language. It traditionally encompasses semantics , syntax , and phonology . Synchronic linguistic st...
-
Of course after Wednesday is Thursday... I had morsyn class. It was 7:00. I did not finish my homework because too many exercises that has ...
-
Beautiful Wednesday I woke up too lately this morning.. But i did not leave my class, advance listening. I got number 4 to practice, it w...
Labels
- Daily Journal (17)
- Weekly Journal (14)
Categorize
- Daily Journal (17)
- Weekly Journal (14)
Sri Indah Rahmiati Nst
Pengikut
Diberdayakan oleh Blogger.